One of the most compelling aspects of theories of democratic deliberation is their answers to the question: where does democratic legitimacy come from? The consent of the governed, deliberation theorists argue, comes from the understanding that emerges from interactions among citizens as they reason together. Empirical research has sought to answer how best to facilitate that reasoning process, how to know when a decision is sufficiently reasoned, and how procedures and institutions can encourage well-reasoned decisions. Yet normative accounts of deliberative democracy (e.g. Chambers 1996; Cohen 1989; Dewey 1927; Habermas 1996; Landemore 2013) rest to a considerable extent on psychological processes such as learning, perspective-taking, and attitude change. Something happens to citizens when they deliberate that does not happen when they watch the news, vote, or protest. This special issue builds on foundational research into such processes by presenting six new articles advancing theory, empirical inquiry, and practice in those areas.

Early theories of deliberation excluded or downplayed aspects of collective understanding influenced by affective-oriented speech. Sanders (1997), Young (1996, 2000), and others argued for the importance of emotions, passions, and affective processes such as sympathy and empathy for deliberation (Barnes 2008; Chambers 2009; Dryzek 2010; Fleckenstein 2007; Goodin 2003; Hall 2005; Krause 2008; Morrell 2010; Thompson & Hoggett 2001). Delving into these topics moved deliberative theory away from a relatively sanitary perspective that a reasoned outcome is forged solely from disciplined argument, toward a messier view acknowledging the role that features such as goals, group and individual identity, and subjective positionality play in constituting reason. We hope that this special collection of articles advances a conceptualization of deliberation as the process of how people understand and relate to one another. To orient readers to this endeavor, this introductory essay summarizes major research findings on psychological dimensions of deliberation, explains recent developments, and outlines the articles in this special issue.

Key Findings from Previous Research

Psychological phenomena in deliberation have long been a focus of inquiry in deliberative democracy, as documented in several excellent overviews (e.g. Gastil 2018; Karpowitz & Mendelberg 2018; Lupia et al. 2012; Mendelberg 2002; Myers & Mendelberg 2013; Pincock 2012). This section summarizes selected major findings from the initial phases of inquiry into psychological aspects of deliberation, with an emphasis on topics directly related to articles in this special issue.

One of the hallmarks of a deliberative democracy is a well-informed public. Deliberative participation improves issue-specific knowledge (Barabas 2004; Fishkin 2009; Min 2007) and understanding of others’ issue-related arguments (Cappella et al. 2002). The public can become more knowledgeable and less misinformed vicariously when policy analyses of deliberating citizens are publicly distributed (Gastil & Knobloch 2020; Reedy et al. 2021). Deliberative participation improves understanding beyond mere knowledge by conferring greater coherence (Gastil & Dillard 1999) or single-peakedness (Farrar et al. 2010; List et al. 2013) of policy attitudes, and facilitated group deliberation fosters attitude changes (Blais et al. 2008; Farrar et al. 2010).

Scholars have explored how issue framing influences citizens’ beliefs and attitudes (Chong & Druckman 2007). Participants who have been exposed to different framings are more likely to express cross-cutting views, and deliberative discussion can neutralize frames’ effects on participants’ issue-attitudes, but it does not if all participants are exposed to the same frame (Druckman 2004; Druckman & Nelson 2003).

A group-level psychological phenomenon of note for deliberative research is group polarization, small-groups’ tendency to intensify their initial issue-attitudes if engaging in unstructured discussion (Sunstein 2002). Studies show the use of facilitation, clear procedural rules reflecting deliberative norms, and balanced informational materials tends to prevent group polarization (e.g. Fishkin et al. 2010; Luskin et al. 2002; Grönlund et al. 2017; Strandberg et al. 2019); deliberations lacking these features tend to exhibit group polarization (e.g. Schkade et al. 2007).

The most developed early research concerning emotions and deliberation drew from Affective Intelligence Theory (Marcus, Neuman & MacKuen 2000). Utilizing studies of citizen deliberation in the public sphere, Marcus, MacKuen, Wolak, and Keele argue that anxiety likely increases deliberation among citizens; enthusiasm may increase participation but likely not deliberation; and loathing, anger, and aversion lead citizens to resist new information and deliberation (MacKuen et al. 2010; Marcus 2002; Wolak & Marcus 2007). Subsequent experiments provided preliminary confirming evidence regarding anxiety (McClain 2009), while questioning whether enthusiasm (McClain 2009) and anger (Kim 2016) might also lead to deliberation. Beyond Affective Intelligence, researchers have examined positive and negative emotions in online forums (Sobkowicz & Sobkowicz 2012), emotions in juries (Hickerson & Gastil 2008), and emotionally-laden discourse (Martin 2012) and biographical affect (Komporozos-Athanasiou & Thompson 2015) in a deliberative patient forum.

Recent Developments

Recently, scholars of deliberation have concentrated their research on a wider array of psychological phenomena, including lay conceptualizations of deliberation, emotions, perspective-taking, relational schemata, communication goals, group identity, and social learning. Several works explore lay people’s conceptions of deliberative processes and elements of deliberation, such as listening and sources of policy information.

The idea that deliberation involves a unique type of listening has sparked interesting conversations about how people process information in small group deliberation. For example, Parks (2019: 25) investigated the values that lay people associated with effective listening in dialogue, including openness and ‘critical thinking’. Related work on listening in deliberation includes Scudder’s (2020) and Bourgault’s (2020) conceptualizations of listening within normative deliberative theory, Mansbridge and Latura’s (2016) call to emphasize listening in deliberative theorizing and research to counter political polarization, and the study of Hendriks et al. (2019) identifying different types of citizens’ listening practices during informal deliberation and those practices’ functions in deliberative systems.

Current studies have examined citizens’ perspectives on the types of information that should be included in deliberation. For example, Lind (2020) identified three different lay epistemologies of public decision making, each with its kinds of required knowledge and recipients of that knowledge. Huntington (2019) examined citizens’ perceptions of Internet memes as a source of information in public political dialogue. That analysis of survey data revealed the so-called third-person effect: respondents reported believing that memes influenced the thinking and beliefs of other citizens, rather than themselves. Studies like Lind’s and Huntington’s explore ways that citizens’ mental models of public deliberation and its components can shape their expectations of deliberative processes.

Recent years have seen an uptick in studies on emotions and deliberation. Neblo (2020) identified twelve roles for emotions in deliberation: normative relevance, motivation to deliberate, inputs, outputs, unmediated inputs, background, enabling conditions, cross check, analogs, application, motivation to act, and struggles for recognition. Saam (2018), using citizen interviews from several deliberative forums, found that disappointment and shame promote exit rather than deliberation and reinforce inequalities because higher status individuals have higher emotional capital, while hope strengthens everyone’s voice and participation irrespective of emotional capital. Analyzing the transcript of a Citizens’ Initiative Review (CIR), Johnson, Black, and Knobloch (2017) found that the competitive-collaborative structure of the CIR was conducive to emotional expression that contributed to deliberation but still allowed participants, with the assistance of moderators, to remain focused on factual accuracy and producing their written statement. Johnson, Morrell and Black (2019) analyzed participant surveys and observer notes from three further CIRs and concluded that during the four-day deliberation enthusiasm was common throughout, happiness steadily increased, anxiety peaked early, sympathy was moderately present, anger was moderately present and peaked on day three, and sadness was uncommon; they theorize that deliberative procedures were likely key in explaining these results. Suiter et al. (2020) utilized a survey experiment and discovered that even non-participants who read balanced information generated by a CIR had greater affective empathy for the other side of the policy debate.

Related studies have delved into the psychological implications of perspective-taking. Muradova (2020: 645) set out ‘a theory of perspective-taking in deliberation’, holding that the degree to which participants engage in reflection is tied to the degree to which participants view issues and policy options from the point of view of other participants (see also Muradova 2021). Features of formal deliberative processes likely to encourage perspective-taking were ‘the presence of diverse perspectives’ and the sharing of ‘personal stories’ (Muradova 2020: 649). Grönlund, Herne, and Setälä (2017: 457) drew on data from a deliberative mini-public experiment to argue that participation in deliberation could increase ‘outgroup empathy’, which they measured as cognitive perspective-taking. Ugarriza and Nussio (2017: 7) found experimental evidence that discussion procedures designed to promote perspective-taking—by encouraging participants to share personal stories, or engage in ‘perspective-giving’—affected participants’ attitudes towards one another.

Other research has examined the role of cognitive structures concerning interpersonal relationships in deliberation. Brinker (2019) drew on relational framing theory (Dillard et al. 1996) to investigate whether cognitive structures that individuals use to interpret the relational dimension of interpersonal communication influenced participants’ reasoning during deliberation. Results showed that participants’ interpretations of the group’s relational dynamics influenced participants’ judgments about the quality of arguments, their endorsements of those arguments, and the degree to which motivated reasoning affected those judgments.

Recent research has also shed light on how goals shape informal deliberative behavior. Eveland et al. (2011) and Morey and Yamamoto (2020) examined the communication goals that individuals prioritized when engaging in informal political deliberations.

Group identity—often characterized as social identity (Tajfel & Turner 2004)—is another focus of psychological inquiry in deliberation, partly due to evidence that group identity intensifies political polarization (e.g. Mason 2018). Some scholars have investigated group identity as an outcome of deliberative participation (e.g. Felicetti et al. 2012, Knobloch & Gastil 2015). More recent research has explored how group identity influences deliberative outcomes. Strickler (2018), for example, found that group-identity-based political partisanship was significantly associated with reduced deliberative reciprocity—the willingness to judge arguments as reasonable and worth considering—toward political opponents, and increased reciprocity toward co-partisans. Batalha et al. (2019) demonstrated that deliberative participants identified more strongly with the superordinate group of their mini-public—the Australian Citizens’ Parliament—than with the subordinate group of their own electoral district, and concluded that deliberative participants can possess multiple group identities that can each shape attitudinal outcomes of deliberation.

Another social-psychological topic of recent inquiry in deliberation is social learning (Bandura 1986; Patterson 1975–1993; Rotter 1982), which deliberation scholars employ in two senses. The first derives from Bandura’s (1986) theory of social learning as a psychosocial process by which individuals change their thoughts and behavior by observing others’ behavior, and which can promote gains in participants’ clarity of speech, use of non-dominant behavior, awareness of opponents’ reasons, and ‘recognition of opposing values’ (Gastil 2004: 325). A second, broader definition of social learning—as changes in beliefs occurring during group interactions, or collective learning—has been more commonly employed in deliberation research (e.g. Barraclough 2013; Dryzek 2013; Rodela, 2013). For some scholars, social learning more specifically involves acquiring knowledge of the views (Kanra 2009; Nikkels et al. 2021) or values (Kenter et al. 2016; Schusler 2003) of other deliberative participants. Some scholars view social learning as involving particular psychological processes, such as perspective-taking (Kanra 2009; Renson 2020) or processes mediated by individuals’ beliefs (Kenter et al. 2016), such as planned behavior (Ajzen 1991). Scholars argue that social learning can occur across deep social divides (Barraclough 2013; Dryzek 2005), as Kanra (2009) showed among diverse participants in a women’s peace organization in Turkey. Social learning promotes outcomes ranging from ‘common purpose[s]’ (Schusler et al. 2003: 312) to improved attitudes towards outgroup members (Kanra 2009) and greater esteem for the public good (Kenter et al. 2016).

The latest deliberative social-learning scholarship foregrounds two themes. The first is social learning in diverse settings, as in Menon and Hartz-Karp’s (2019) study of social learning in deliberative processes in India and De Vente et al.’s (2016) investigation of social learning during deliberations in 13 nations. The second is more complex models of social learning. For example, Renson (2020) demonstrated social learning concerning beliefs and attitudes among participatory-budgeting participants, while Kenter et al.’s (2016) social-learning model integrates multiple psycho-social processes to explain how participants’ beliefs mediate associations between broad value commitments and value-criteria in deliberation.

Articles in This Special Issue

The articles in this special issue offer fresh theoretical and empirical insights on several recent topics concerning psychological aspects of deliberation, while extending knowledge on long-standing topics in the field.

Analyzing qualitative data from a CIR about medical-marijuana legalization, Fisher et al. uncover new categories of reasoning employed by deliberating citizens. Moreover, Fisher et al. explore associations between those categories—which include states of uncertainty and processes of questioning—and types of expression of disagreement. Khoban employed survey experiments to investigate citizens’ mental models of deliberative interactions during hypothetical mini-publics. Contributing to recent research on citizens’ lay conceptualizations of deliberation as well as social-identity dynamics in deliberative processes, while incorporating concepts from prior research on framing in deliberation, Khoban explores whether cues about the social groups to be included in a prospective mini-public influenced citizens’ expectations concerning deliberation.

Also building on recent research concerning social identity in deliberation, Wright theorizes about the threat that social identity poses to persuasion-based accounts of democratic deliberation. Wright draws on principles from Mary Parker Follett’s deliberative theory to set out a new framework for citizen deliberation based on the creative development of mutually beneficial solutions.

Two articles provide fresh insights on emotions in deliberation. Replicating an earlier study (Johnson et al. 2019), Morrell et al. discover evidence from three CIRs confirming their findings that deliberative procedures influence participants’ emotions at different stages of the deliberative process in a mini-public. Providing a practitioner perspective on emotions in deliberation, Stains and Sarrouf explain how the experience of powerful emotions—especially those arising from polarizing social identities—can inhibit deliberation. For these authors, structured dialogue procedures can enable citizens to control such emotions and enhance their sense of agency in preparation for constructive participation in deliberative decision making.

Extending recent scholarship on goals in informal deliberation, Richards and Neblo’s theoretical model explains how formal and informal deliberative contexts influence citizens’ communicative goals, which in turn shape citizens’ reason-giving behavior. Communicative plans—activated during processes of information seeking and sense-making—are expected to mediate associations between goals and communicative practices.

Apprehending how people understand and relate to one another is critical to the political psychology of deliberation. Considered together, the articles in this special issue highlight innovative theorizing and empirical research in this area and illuminate paths of further inquiry in this vital domain.

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

References

1 Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179–211. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

2 Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall.

3 Barabas, J. (2004). How deliberation affects policy opinions. American Political Science Review, 98(4), 687–701. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055404041425

4 Barnes, M. (2008). Passionate participation: Emotional experiences and expressions in deliberative forums. Critical Social Policy, 28(4), 461–481. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0261018308095280

5 Barraclough, R. (2013). Social learning and deliberative democracy. In L. Shultz & T. Kajner (Eds.), Engaged scholarship: The politics of engagement and disengagement (pp. 107–120). Sense Publishers. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-6209-290-7_8

6 Batalha, L., Niemeyer, S., Dryzek, J. S., & Gastil, J. (2019). Psychological mechanisms of deliberative transformation: The role of group identity. Journal of Public Deliberation, 15(1), Article 2. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.16997/jdd.313

7 Blais, A., Carty, R. K., & Fournier, P. (2008). Do citizens’ assemblies make reasoned choices? In M. E. Warren & H. Pearse (Eds.), Designing deliberative democracy: The British Columbia Citizens’ Assembly (pp. 127–144). Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511491177.008

8 Bourgault, S. (2020). Democratic practice and ‘caring to deliberate’: A Gadamerian account of conversation and listening. In P. Urban & L. Ward (Eds.), Care ethics, democratic citizenship and the state (pp. 31–51). Springer International/Palgrave Macmillan. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-41437-5_2

9 Brinker, D. L. (2019). Biased political reasoning and relational inferences in a small-group deliberative context. Communication Quarterly, 67(2), 221–241. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/01463373.2019.1573202

10 Cappella, J. N., Price, V., & Nir, L. (2002). Argument repertoire as a reliable and valid measure of opinion quality: Electronic dialogue during campaign 2000. Political Communication, 19, 73–93. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/105846002317246498

11 Chambers, S. (1996). Reasonable democracy: Jürgen Habermas and the politics of discourse. Cornell University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7591/9781501722547

12 Chambers, S. (2009). Rhetoric and the public sphere: Has deliberative democracy abandoned mass democracy? Political Theory, 37(3): 323–350. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0090591709332336

13 Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.polisci.10.072805.103054

14 Cohen, J. (1989). Deliberation and democratic legitimacy. In A. Hamlin & P. Pettit (Eds.), The good polity (pp. 17–34). Blackwell.

15 De Vente, J., Reed, M. S., Stringer, L. C., Valente, S., & Newig, J. (2016). How does the context and design of participatory decision making processes affect their outcomes? Evidence from sustainable land management in global drylands. Ecology and Society, 21(2), 24. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.5751/ES-08053-210224

16 Dewey, J. (1927). The public and its problems. Holt.

17 Dillard, J. P., Solomon, D. H., & Samp, J. A. (1996). Framing social reality: The relevance of relational judgments. Communication Research, 23(6), 703–723. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/009365096023006004

18 Druckman, J. N. (2004). Political preference formation: Competition, deliberation, and the (ir)relevance of framing effects. American Political Science Review, 98(4), 671–686. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055404041413

19 Druckman, J. N., & Nelson, K. R. (2003). Framing and deliberation: How citizens’ conversations limit elite influence. American Journal of Political Science, 47(4), 729–745. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/1540-5907.00051

20 Dryzek, J. S. (2005). Deliberative democracy in divided societies: Alternatives to agonism and analgesia. Political Theory, 33(2), 218–242. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0090591704268372

21 Dryzek, J. S. (2010). Rhetoric in democracy: A systemic appreciation. Political Theory, 38(3), 319–339. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0090591709359596

22 Dryzek, J. S. (2013). The politics of the Earth: Environmental discourses (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.

23 Eveland, W. P., Morey, A. C., & Hutchens, M. J. (2011). Beyond deliberation: New directions for the study of informal political conversation from a communication perspective. Journal of Communication, 61(6), 1082–1103. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01598.x

24 Farrar, C., Fishkin, J. S., Green, D. P., List, C., Luskin, R. C., & Levy Paluck, E. (2010). Disaggregating deliberation’s effects: An experiment within a deliberative poll. British Journal of Political Science, 40(2), 333–347. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123409990433

25 Felicetti, A., Gastil, J., Hartz-Karp, J., & Carson, L. (2012). Collective identity and voice at the Australian Citizens’ Parliament. Journal of Public Deliberation, 8(1), Article 5. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.16997/jdd.127

26 Fishkin, J. S. (2009). When the people speak: Deliberative democracy and public consultation. Oxford University Press.

27 Fishkin, J. S., He, B., Luskin, R. C., & Siu, A. (2010). Deliberative democracy in an unlikely place: Deliberative polling in China. British Journal of Political Science, 40, 435–448. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123409990330

28 Fleckenstein, K. S. (2007). Once again with feeling: Empathy in deliberative discourse. JAC 27(3/4), 701–716.

29 Gastil, J. (2004). Adult civic education through the National Issues Forums: Developing democratic habits and dispositions through public deliberation. Adult Education Quarterly, 54(4), 308–328. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0741713604266142

30 Gastil, J. (2018). The lessons and limitations of experiments in democratic deliberation. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 14, 271–291. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-lawsocsci-110316-113639

31 Gastil, J., & Dillard, J. P. (1999). Increasing political sophistication through public deliberation. Political Communication, 16, 3–23. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/105846099198749

32 Gastil, J., & Knobloch, K. R. (2020). Hope for democracy: How citizens can bring reason back into politics. Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190084523.001.0001

33 Goodin, R. E. (2003). Reflective democracy. Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/0199256179.001.0001

34 Grönlund, K., Herne, K., & Setälä, M. (2017). Empathy in a citizen deliberation experiment. Scandinavian Political Studies, 40(4), 457–480. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9477.12103

35 Habermas, J. (1996). Between facts and norms: Contributions to a discourse theory of law and democracy (W. Rehg, Trans.). MIT Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/1564.001.0001

36 Hall, C. (2005). The trouble with passion: Political theory beyond the reign of reason. Routledge.

37 Hendriks, C. M., Ercan, S. A., & Duus, S. (2019). Listening in polarised controversies: A study of listening practices in the public sphere. Policy Sciences, 52(1), 137–151. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/s11077-018-9343-3

38 Hickerson, A., & Gastil, J. (2008). Assessing the difference critique of deliberation: Gender, emotion, and the jury experience. Communication Theory, 18(2), 281–303. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00323.x

39 Huntington, H. E. (2019, November). ‘People think they know more than they actually know’: An explanation of audience perceptions about the proper place of internet memes within public dialogue about politics. Paper presented at the National Communication Association Annual Convention, Baltimore, MD, United States.

40 Johnson, G. F., Black, L. W., & Knobloch, K. R. (2017). Citizens’ initiative review process: Mediating emotions, promoting productive deliberation. Policy and Politics, 45(3), 431–447. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1332/030557316X14595273846060

41 Johnson, G. F., Morrell, M. E., & Black, L. W. (2019). Emotions and deliberation in the Citizens’ Initiative Review. Social Science Quarterly, 100(6), 2168–2187. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/ssqu.12707

42 Kanra, B. (2009). Islam, democracy and dialogue in Turkey: Deliberating in divided societies. Routledge. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315589985

43 Karpowitz, C. F., & Mendelberg, T. (2018). The political psychology of deliberation. In A. Bächtiger, J. S. Dryzek, J. Mansbridge, & M. E. Warren (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of deliberative democracy (pp. 535–555). Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198747369.013.36

44 Kenter, J. O., Reed, M. S., & Fazey, I. (2016). The deliberative value formation model. Ecosystem Services, 21, 194–207. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2016.09.015

45 Kim, N. (2016). Beyond rationality: The role of anger and information in deliberation. Communication Research, 43(1), 3–24. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0093650213510943

46 Knobloch, K. R., & Gastil, J. (2015). Civic (re)socialization: The educative effects of deliberative participation. Politics, 35(2), 183–200. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9256.12069

47 Komporozos-Athanasiou, A., & Thompson, M. (2015). The role of emotion in enabling and conditioning public deliberation outcomes: A sociological investigation. Public Administration, 93(4), 1138–1151. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/padm.12188

48 Krause, S. R. (2008). Civil passions: Moral sentiment and deliberation. Princeton University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9781400837281

49 Landemore, H. (2013). Democratic reason: Politics, collective intelligence, and the rule of the many. Princeton University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.23943/princeton/9780691155654.001.0001

50 Lind, C. (2020). Everyday epistemologies: What people say about knowledge and what it means for public deliberation. Journal of Public Deliberation, 15(3), Article 7. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.16997/jdd.345

51 List, C., Luskin, R. C., Fishkin, J. S., & McLean, I. (2013). Deliberation, single-peakedness, and the possibility of meaningful democracy: Evidence from deliberative polls. Journal of Politics, 75(1), 80–95. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0022381612000886

52 Lupia, A., Krupnikov, Y., & Levine, A. S. (2012). Beyond facts and norms: How psychological transparency threatens and restores deliberation’s legitimating potential. Southern California Law Review, 86, 459–494.

53 Luskin, R. C., Fishkin, J. S., & Jowell, R. (2002). Considered opinions: Deliberative polling in Britain. British Journal of Political Science, 32(3), 455–487. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123402000194

54 MacKuen, M., Wolak, J., Keele, L., & Marcus, G. E. (2010). Civic engagements: Resolute partisanship or reflective deliberation. American Journal of Political Science, 54(2), 440–458. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2010.00440.x

55 Mansbridge, J., & Latura, A. (2016). The polarization crisis in the US and the future of listening. In T. Norris (Ed.), Strong democracy in crisis: Promise or peril? (pp. 29–54). Lexington Books.

56 Marcus, G. E. (2002). The sentimental citizen: Emotion in democratic politics. Pennsylvania State University Press.

57 Marcus, G. E., Neuman, W. R., & MacKuen, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and political judgment. University of Chicago Press.

58 Martin, G. P. (2012). Public deliberation in action: Emotion, inclusion and exclusion in participatory decision making. Critical Social Policy, 32(2), 163–183. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0261018311420276

59 Mason, L. (2018). Uncivil agreement: How politics became our identity. University of Chicago Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226524689.001.0001

60 McClain, C. (2009). Debating restrictions on embryonic stem cell research. Politics and the Life Sciences, 28(2), 48–68. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.2990/28_2_48

61 Mendelberg, T. (2002). The deliberative citizen: Theory and evidence. In R. Y. Shapiro & M. X. Delli Carpini (Eds.), Political decision-making, deliberation and participation (pp. 151–193). Elsevier Science.

62 Menon, S., & Hartz-Karp, J. (2019). Linking traditional ‘organic’ and ‘induced’ public participation with deliberative democracy: Experiments in Pune, India. Journal of Education for Sustainable Development, 13(2), 193–214. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0973408219874959

63 Min, S.-J. (2007). Online vs. face-to-face deliberation: Effects on civic engagement. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1369–1387. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00377.x

64 Morrell, M. E. (2010). Empathy and democracy: Feeling, thinking, and deliberation. Pennsylvania State University Press.

65 Morey, A. C., & Yamamoto, M. (2020). Exploring political discussion motivations: Relationships with different forms of political talk. Communication Studies, 71(1), 78–97. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/10510974.2019.1692885

66 Muradova, L. (2020). Seeing the other side? Perspective-taking and reflective political judgements in interpersonal deliberation. Political Studies, 69(3): 644–664. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0032321720916605

67 Muradova, L. (2021). Reasoning across the divide? Interpersonal deliberation, emotions and reflective political judgements. PhD Thesis. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/retrieve/612059

68 Myers, C. D., & Mendelberg, T. (2013). Political deliberation. In L. Huddy, D. O. Sears, & J. S. Levy (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of political psychology (2nd ed., pp. 699–734). Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199760107.013.0022

69 Neblo, M. A. (2020). Impassioned democracy: The roles of emotion in deliberative theory. American Political Science Review, 114(3), 923–927. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055420000210

70 Nikkels, M. J., Leith, P., Kumar, S., Mendham, N., & Dewulf, A. (2021). The social learning potential of participatory water valuation workshops: A case study in Tasmania, Australia. Environmental Policy and Governance, 31(5), 474–491. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1002/eet.1939

71 Parks, E. S. (2019). The ethics of listening: Creating space for sustainable dialogue. Lexington Books.

72 Patterson, G. R. (1975–1993). A social learning approach to family intervention. Castalia Publishing Company.

73 Pincock, H. (2012). Does deliberation make better citizens? In T. Nabatchi, J. Gastil, G. M. Weiksner, & M. Leighinger (Eds.), Democracy in motion: Evaluating the practice and impact of deliberative civic engagement (pp. 135–162). Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199899265.003.0007

74 Reedy, J., Anderson, C., & Conte, P. (2021). Citizen deliberation as a correction: The role of deliberative mini-publics in addressing political misperceptions. In D. C. Barker & E. Suhay (Eds.), The politics of truth in polarized America (pp. 384–397). Oxford University Press.

75 Renson, T. (2020). Deliberation out of the laboratory into democracy:Quasi-experimental research on deliberative opinions in Antwerp’s participatory budgeting. Politics of the Low Countries, 2(1), 3–27. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.5553/PLC/258999292020002001002

76 Rodela, R. (2013). The social learning discourse: Trends, themes and interdisciplinary influences in current research. Environmental Science & Policy, 25, 157–166. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.envsci.2012.09.002

77 Rotter, J. B. (1982). Social learning theory. In N. T. Feather (Ed.), Expectations and actions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp. 241–260). Erlbaum. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.4324/9781003150879-12

78 Saam, N. J. (2018). Recognizing the emotion work in deliberation. Why emotions do not make deliberative democracy more democratic. Political Psychology, 39(4), 755–774. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12461

79 Sanders, L. M. (1997). Against deliberation. Political Theory, 25(3), 347–376. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0090591797025003002

80 Scudder, M. F. (2020). Beyond empathy and inclusion: The challenge of listening in democratic deliberation. Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780197535455.001.0001

81 Schkade, D., Sunstein, C. R., & Hastie, R. (2007). What happened on deliberation day? California Law Review, 95, 915–940. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.911646

82 Schusler, T. M., Decker, D. J., & Pfeffer, M. J. (2003). Social learning for collaborative natural resource management. Society & Natural Resources, 16(4), 309–326. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1080/08941920390178874

83 Sobkowicz, P., & Sobkowicz, A. (2012). Two-year study of emotion and communication patterns in a highly polarized political discussion forum. Social Science Computer Review, 30(4), 448–469. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0894439312436512

84 Strandberg, K., Himmelroos, S., & Grönlund, K. (2019). Do discussions in like-minded groups necessarily lead to more extreme opinions? Deliberative democracy and group polarization. International Political Science Review, 40(1), 41–57. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0192512117692136

85 Strickler, R. (2018). Deliberate with the enemy? Polarization, social identity, and attitudes toward disagreement. Political Research Quarterly, 71(1), 3–18. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/1065912917721371

86 Suiter, J., Muradova, L., Gastil, J., & Farrell, D. M. (2020). Scaling up deliberation: Testing the potential of mini-publics to enhance the deliberative capacity of citizens. Swiss Political Science Review, 26(3), 253–272. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/spsr.12405

87 Sunstein, C. R. (2002). The law of group polarization. Journal of Political Philosophy, 10(2), 175–195. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9760.00148

88 Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. (2004). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In M. J. Hatch & M. Schultz (Eds.), Organizational identity: A reader (pp. 56–65). Oxford University Press.

89 Thompson, S., & Hoggett, P. (2001). The emotional dynamics of deliberative democracy. Policy and Politics, 29(3), 351–364. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1332/0305573012501396

90 Ugarriza, J. E., & Nussio, E. (2017). The effect of perspective-taking on postconflict reconciliation. An experimental approach. Political Psychology, 38(1), 3–19. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12324

91 Wolak, J., & Marcus, G. E. (2007). Personality and emotional response: Strategic and tactical responses to changing political circumstances. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 614(1), 172–195. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0002716207306086

92 Young, I. M. (1996). Communication and the other: Beyond deliberative democracy. In S. Benhabib (Ed.), Democracy and difference (pp. 120–135). Princeton University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9780691234168-007

93 Young, I. M. (2000). Inclusion and democracy. Oxford University Press.